الرصد بالصدى عند الحيوانات

(تم التحويل من Animal echolocation)

الرصد بالصدى (إنگليزية: Echolocation، ويسمى أيضاً السونار الحيوي (إنگليزية: bio sonar)، هو سونار بيولوجي تستخدمه العديد من أنواع الحيوان. يصدر صدى الحيوانات نداءات إلى البيئة وتستمع إلى أصداء تلك الأصوات التي تعود من أشياء مختلفة بالقرب منها. يستخدمون هذه الأصداء لتحديد الأشياء. يستخدم تحديد الموقع بالصدى من أجل الملاحة والبحث عن الطعام والصيد في بيئات مختلفة. تشمل الحيوانات التي ترصد بالصدى بعض الثدييات (وأبرزها وحوش لوراسية) وعدد قليل من الطيور. خاصةً بعض أنواع الخفافيش والحوتيات المسننة (الحيتان والدلافين ذات الأسنان)، ولكن أيضاً في أشكال أبسط في مجموعات أخرى مثل الزبابات ومجموعتين من الطيور التي تعيش في الكهوف، والتي تسمى السمامات الكهفية من جنس سميمة العش (المعروف سابقاً باسم ملتصقة الأعشاش) و طائر الزيت الكاريبي من فصيلة طيور الزيت. [1][2]

تصوير إشارات الموجات فوق الصوتية المنبعثة من خفاش وصدى من جسم قريب

. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

الأبحاث المبكرة

تمت صياغة مصطلح الرصد بالصدى عند الحيوانات في عام 1938 من قبل عالم الحيوان الأمريكي دونالد گرفن، والذي أوضح مع روبرت گالامبوس هذه الظاهرة لأول مرة في الخفافيش.[3][4] كما وصف گرفن في كتابه،[5] استنتج العالم الإيطالي من القرن الثامن عشر لادزارو سپالانساني، عن طريق سلسلة من التجارب التفصيلية، أنه عندما تطير الخفافيش في الليل، فإنها تعتمد على بعض الإحساس إلى جانب الرؤية، لكنه لم يكتشف ذلك أما المعنى الآخر فكان الاستماع.[6][7] كرر الطبيب وعالم الطبيعة السويسري لويس جورين تجارب سپالانساني (باستخدام أنواع مختلفة من الخفافيش)، وخلص إلى أنه عندما تصطاد الخفافيش في الليل، فإنها تعتمد على السمع.[8][9][10] في عام 1908، أكد والتر لويس هان اكتشافات سپالانساني وجورين.[11]

في عام 1912، اقترح المخترع هيرام مكسيم بشكل مستقل أن الخفافيش تستخدم الصوت تحت النطاق السمعي البشري لتجنب العقبات.[12] في عام 1920، اقترح عالم وظائف الأعضاء الإنجليزي هاملتون هارتردگ بشكل صحيح أن الخفافيش تستخدم ترددات أعلى من نطاق سمع الإنسان.[13][14]

لم يتم وصف الرصد بالصدى في الحوتيات المسننة (الحيتان ذات الأسنان) بشكل صحيح إلا بعد عقدين من عمل گرفن وگالامبوس، بواسطة شيفيل ومكبرايد في عام 1956.[15] ومع ذلك، في عام 1953، اقترح جاك إيڤ كوستو في كتابه الأول، العالم الصامت،[16]أن خنازير البحر لديها شيء مثل السونار، بناءً على قدراتهم الملاحية.


المبدأ

الرصد بالصدى هو نفسه السونار النشط، باستخدام الأصوات التي يصدرها الحيوان نفسه. يتم قياس المدى عن طريق قياس التأخير الزمني بين انبعاث صوت الحيوان وأي صدى عائد من البيئة. توفر الشدة النسبية للصوت الذي يتم استقباله في كل أذن بالإضافة إلى التأخير الزمني بين الوصول إلى الأذنين معلومات حول الزاوية الأفقية (السمت) التي تصل منها الموجات الصوتية المنعكسة.[17]

على عكس بعض السونارات التي يصنعها الإنسان والتي تعتمد على العديد من الحزم الضيقة للغاية والعديد من أجهزة الاستقبال لتحديد الهدف (مسبار الصدى متعدد الحزم)، فإن الرصد بالصدى للحيوان يحتوي على جهاز إرسال واحد وجهازين استقبال (الأذنين) متباعدتين قليلاً. تصل الأصداء التي تعود إلى الأذنين في أوقات مختلفة وبكثافة مختلفة، اعتماداً على موضع الجسم الذي يولّد الصدى. تستخدم الحيوانات اختلافات الوقت والجهارة لإدراك المسافة والاتجاه. مع تحديد الموقع بالصدى، يمكن للخفاش أو أي حيوان آخر أن يرى ليس فقط إلى أين يتجه ولكن أيضاً إلى أي مدى يوجد حيوان آخر ونوعه وميزات أخرى.[18][19]

في أبسط المستويات، يعتمد الرصد بالصدى على التشريح العصبي لدوائر الدماغ السمعية. في الأساس، تسمح مسارات الدماغ الصاعدة في جذع الدماغ للدماغ بحساب الفرق بين الأذنين إلى أجزاء صغيرة جداً من الثانية.[20]

الخفافيش

 
صورة طيفية لصوت خفاش Pipistrellus pipistrellus أثناء اقتراب الفريسة. يغطي التسجيل ما مجموعه 1.1 ثانية؛ التردد الرئيسي المنخفض 45 كيلوهرتز (كما هو الحال في Common pipistrelle). حوالي 150 مللي ثانية قبل وقت النداء النهائي وأصبحت مدة النداءات أقصر بكثير ("تغذية الطنين").
Corresponding audio file:

تستخدم الخفافيش الرصد بالصدى من أجل التنقل والأعلاف، غالباً في الظلام الدامس. يخرجون عموماً من مجاثمهم في الكهوف أو السندرات أو الأشجار عند الغسق ويصطادون الحشرات في الليل. باستخدام الرصد بالصدى، يمكن للخفافيش تحديد مدى بُعد الكائن، وحجمه وشكله وكثافته، والاتجاه (إن وجد) الذي يتحرك به الكائن. يسمح استخدامهم للرصد بالصدى، جنباً إلى جنب مع الطيران، إلى احتلالهم المكان الذي يوجد فيه غالباً العديد من الحشرات (التي تخرج في الليل نظراً لوجود عدد أقل من الحيوانات المفترسة في ذلك الوقت)، ومنافسة أقل على الطعام، وعدداً أقل من الأنواع التي قد تفترس الخفافيش نفسها.[21]

Echolocating bats generate ultrasound via the larynx and emit the sound through the open mouth or, much more rarely, the nose.[22] The latter is most pronounced in the horseshoe bats (Rhinolophus spp.). Bat echolocation calls range in frequency from 14,000 to well over 100,000 Hz, mostly beyond the range of the human ear (typical human hearing range is considered to be from 20 Hz to 20,000 Hz). Bats may estimate the elevation of targets by interpreting the interference patterns caused by the echoes reflecting from the tragus, a flap of skin in the external ear.[23]

There are two hypotheses about the evolution of echolocation in bats. The first suggests that laryngeal echolocation evolved twice, or more, in Chiroptera, at least once in the Yangochiroptera and at least once in the horseshoe bats (Rhinolophidae).[24][25][26] The second proposes that laryngeal echolocation had a single origin in Chiroptera, was subsequently lost in the family Pteropodidae.[27][28] Later, the genus Rousettus in the Pteropodidae family developed a different mechanism of echolocation using a system of tongue-clicking.[29]

Individual bat species echolocate within specific frequency ranges that suit their environment and prey types. This has sometimes been used by researchers to identify bats flying in an area simply by recording their calls with ultrasonic recorders known as "bat detectors". However echolocation calls are not always species specific and some bats overlap in the type of calls they use so recordings of echolocation calls cannot be used to identify all bats. In recent years researchers in several countries have developed "bat call libraries" that contain recordings of local bat species that have been identified known as "reference calls" to assist with identification.[30] [31] [32]

Since the 1970s there has been an ongoing controversy among researchers as to whether bats use a form of processing known from radar termed coherent cross-correlation. Coherence means that the phase of the echolocation signals is used by the bats, while cross-correlation just implies that the outgoing signal is compared with the returning echoes in a running process. Today most – but not all – researchers believe that they use cross-correlation, but in an incoherent form, termed a filter bank receiver.[بحاجة لمصدر]

When searching for prey they produce sounds at a low rate (10–20 clicks/second). During the search phase the sound emission is coupled to respiration, which is again coupled to the wingbeat. This coupling appears to dramatically conserve energy as there is little to no additional energetic cost of echolocation to flying bats.[33] After detecting a potential prey item, echolocating bats increase the rate of pulses, ending with the terminal buzz, at rates as high as 200 clicks/second. During approach to a detected target, the duration of the sounds is gradually decreased, as is the energy of the sound.[34]

Calls and ecology

Echolocating bats occupy a diverse set of ecological conditions ; they can be found living in environments as different as Europe and Madagascar, and hunting for food sources as different as insects, frogs, nectar, fruit, and blood. Additionally, the characteristics of an echolocation call are adapted to the particular environment, hunting behavior, and food source of the particular bat. However, this adaptation of echolocation calls to ecological factors is constrained by the phylogenetic relationship of the bats, leading to a process known as descent with modification, and resulting in the diversity of the Chiroptera today.[35][36][37][38][39][40]

Flying insects are a common source of food for echolocating bats and some insects (moths in particular) can hear the calls of predatory bats. There is evidence that moth hearing has evolved in response to bat echolocation to avoid capture.[41] Furthermore, these moth adaptations provide selective pressure for bats to improve their insect-hunting systems and this cycle culminates in a moth-bat "evolutionary arms race."[42][43]

Acoustic features

Describing the diversity of bat echolocation calls requires examination of the frequency and temporal features of the calls. It is the variations in these aspects that produce echolocation calls suited for different acoustic environments and hunting behaviors.[35][37][40][44][45]

Frequency

Bat call frequencies range from as low as 11 kHz to as high as 212 kHz.[46] Insectivorous aerial-hawking bats have a call frequency between 20 kHz and 60 kHz because it is the frequency that gives the best range and image acuity and makes them less conspicuous to insects.[47] However, low frequencies are adaptive for some species with different prey and environments. Euderma maculatum, a species that feeds on moths, uses a particularly low frequency of 12.7 kHz that cannot be heard by moths.[48]

Frequency modulation and constant frequency

Echolocation calls can be composed of two different types of frequency structure: frequency modulated (FM) sweeps, and constant frequency (CF) tones. A particular call can consist of one, the other, or both structures. An FM sweep is a broadband signal – that is, it contains a downward sweep through a range of frequencies. A CF tone is a narrowband signal: the sound stays constant at one frequency throughout its duration.[بحاجة لمصدر]

Intensity

Echolocation calls have been measured at intensities anywhere between 60 and 140 decibels.[49] Certain bat species can modify their call intensity mid-call, lowering the intensity as they approach objects that reflect sound strongly. This prevents the returning echo from deafening the bat.[45] High-intensity calls such as those from aerial-hawking bats (133 dB) are adaptive to hunting in open skies. Their high intensity calls are necessary to even have moderate detection of surroundings because air has a high absorption of ultrasound and because insects' size only provide a small target for sound reflection.[50] Additionally, the so-called "whispering bats" have adapted low-amplitude echolocation so that their prey, moths, which are able to hear echolocation calls, are less able to detect and avoid an oncoming bat.[48]


. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

Harmonic composition

Calls can be composed of one frequency or multiple frequencies comprising a harmonic series. In the latter case, the call is usually dominated by a certain harmonic ("dominant" frequencies are those present at higher intensities than other harmonics present in the call).[بحاجة لمصدر]

Call duration

A single echolocation call (a call being a single continuous trace on a sound spectrogram, and a series of calls comprising a sequence or pass) can last anywhere from 0.2 to 100 milliseconds in duration, depending on the stage of prey-catching behavior that the bat is engaged in. For example, the duration of a call usually decreases when the bat is in the final stages of prey capture – this enables the bat to call more rapidly without overlap of call and echo. Reducing duration comes at the cost of having less total sound available for reflecting off objects and being heard by the bat.[46]

Pulse interval

The time interval between subsequent echolocation calls (or pulses) determines two aspects of a bat's perception. First, it establishes how quickly the bat's auditory scene information is updated. For example, bats increase the repetition rate of their calls (that is, decrease the pulse interval) as they home in on a target. This allows the bat to get new information regarding the target's location at a faster rate when it needs it most. Secondly, the pulse interval determines the maximum range that bats can detect objects. This is because bats can only keep track of the echoes from one call at a time; as soon as they make another call they stop listening for echoes from the previously made call. For example, a pulse interval of 100 ms (typical of a bat searching for insects) allows sound to travel in air roughly 34 meters so a bat can only detect objects as far away as 17 meters (the sound has to travel out and back). With a pulse interval of 5 ms (typical of a bat in the final moments of a capture attempt), the bat can only detect objects up to 85 cm away. Therefore, the bat constantly has to make a choice between getting new information updated quickly and detecting objects far away.[51]

FM signal advantages

Echolocation call produced by Pipistrellus pipistrellus, an FM bat. The ultrasonic call has been "heterodyned" – multiplied by a constant frequency to produce frequency subtraction, and thus an audible sound – by a bat detector. A key feature of the recording is the increase in the repetition rate of the call as the bat nears its target – this is called the "terminal buzz".

The major advantage conferred by an FM signal is extremely precise range discrimination, or localization, of the target. J.A. Simmons demonstrated this effect with a series of elegant experiments that showed how bats using FM signals could distinguish between two separate targets even when the targets were less than half a millimeter apart. This ability is due to the broadband sweep of the signal, which allows for better resolution of the time delay between the call and the returning echo, thereby improving the cross correlation of the two. Additionally, if harmonic frequencies are added to the FM signal, then this localization becomes even more precise.[35][36][37][40]

One possible disadvantage of the FM signal is a decreased operational range of the call. Because the energy of the call is spread out among many frequencies, the distance at which the FM-bat can detect targets is limited.[38] This is in part because any echo returning at a particular frequency can only be evaluated for a brief fraction of a millisecond, as the fast downward sweep of the call does not remain at any one frequency for long.[36]

CF signal advantages

The structure of a CF signal is adaptive in that it allows the CF-bat to detect both the velocity of a target, and the fluttering of a target's wings as Doppler shifted frequencies. A Doppler shift is an alteration in sound wave frequency, and is produced in two relevant situations: when the bat and its target are moving relative to each other, and when the target's wings are oscillating back and forth. CF-bats must compensate for Doppler shifts, lowering the frequency of their call in response to echoes of elevated frequency – this ensures that the returning echo remains at the frequency to which the ears of the bat are most finely tuned. The oscillation of a target's wings also produces amplitude shifts, which gives a CF-bat additional help in distinguishing a flying target from a stationary one.[52][37][40][36][39][35]

Additionally, because the signal energy of a CF call is concentrated into a narrow frequency band, the operational range of the call is much greater than that of an FM signal. This relies on the fact that echoes returning within the narrow frequency band can be summed over the entire length of the call, which maintains a constant frequency for up to 100 milliseconds.[36][38]

Acoustic environments of FM and CF signals

A frequency modulated (FM) component is excellent for hunting prey while flying in close, cluttered environments. Two aspects of the FM signal account for this fact: the precise target localization conferred by the broadband signal, and the short duration of the call. The first of these is essential because in a cluttered environment, the bats must be able to resolve their prey from large amounts of background noise. The 3D localization abilities of the broadband signal enable the bat to do exactly that, providing it with what Simmons and Stein (1980) call a "clutter rejection strategy." This strategy is further improved by the use of harmonics, which, as previously stated, enhance the localization properties of the call. The short duration of the FM call is also best in close, cluttered environments because it enables the bat to emit many calls extremely rapidly without overlap. This means that the bat can get an almost continuous stream of information – essential when objects are close, because they will pass by quickly – without confusing which echo corresponds to which call.[39][40][35][38]

A constant frequency (CF) component is often used by bats hunting for prey while flying in open, clutter-free environments, or by bats that wait on perches for their prey to appear. The success of the former strategy is due to two aspects of the CF call, both of which confer excellent prey-detection abilities. First, the greater working range of the call allows bats to detect targets present at great distances – a common situation in open environments. Second, the length of the call is also suited for targets at great distances: in this case, there is a decreased chance that the long call will overlap with the returning echo. The latter strategy is made possible by the fact that the long, narrowband call allows the bat to detect Doppler shifts, which would be produced by an insect moving either towards or away from a perched bat.[39][40][35][38]

Neural mechanisms

Because bats use echolocation to orient themselves and to locate objects, their auditory systems are adapted for this purpose, highly specialized for sensing and interpreting the stereotyped echolocation calls characteristic of their own species. This specialization is evident from the inner ear up to the highest levels of information processing in the auditory cortex.[53]

Inner ear and primary sensory neurons

Both CF and FM bats have specialized inner ears which allow them to hear sounds in the ultrasonic range, far outside the range of human hearing. Although in most other aspects, the bat's auditory organs are similar to those of most other mammals, certain bats (horseshoe bats, Rhinolophus spp. and the moustached bat, Pteronotus parnelii) with a constant frequency (CF) component to their call (known as high duty cycle bats) do have a few additional adaptations for detecting the predominant frequency (and harmonics) of the CF vocalization. These include a narrow frequency "tuning" of the inner ear organs, with an especially large area responding to the frequency of the bat's returning echoes.[39]

The basilar membrane within the cochlea contains the first of these specializations for echo information processing. In bats that use CF signals, the section of the membrane that responds to the frequency of returning echoes is much larger than the region of response for any other frequency. For example, in the greater horseshoe bat, Rhinolophus ferrumequinum, there is a disproportionately lengthened and thickened section of the membrane that responds to sounds around 83 kHz, the constant frequency of the echo produced by the bat's call. This area of high sensitivity to a specific, narrow range of frequency is known as an "acoustic fovea".[54]

Odontocetes (toothed whales and dolphins) have similar cochlear specializations to those found in bats. Odontocetes also have the highest neural investment of any cochleae reported to date with ratios of greater than 1500 ganglion cells/mm of basilar membrane.[بحاجة لمصدر]

Further along the auditory pathway, the movement of the basilar membrane results in the stimulation of primary auditory neurons. Many of these neurons are specifically "tuned" (respond most strongly) to the narrow frequency range of returning echoes of CF calls. Because of the large size of the acoustic fovea, the number of neurons responding to this region, and thus to the echo frequency, is especially high.[55]


. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

Inferior colliculus

In the Inferior colliculus, a structure in the bat's midbrain, information from lower in the auditory processing pathway is integrated and sent on to the auditory cortex. As George Pollak and others showed in a series of papers in 1977, the interneurons in this region have a very high level of sensitivity to time differences, since the time delay between a call and the returning echo tells the bat its distance from the target object. While most neurons respond more quickly to stronger stimuli, collicular neurons maintain their timing accuracy even as signal intensity changes.[بحاجة لمصدر]

These interneurons are specialized for time sensitivity in several ways. First, when activated, they generally respond with only one or two action potentials. This short duration of response allows their action potentials to give a very specific indication of the exact moment of the time when the stimulus arrived, and to respond accurately to stimuli that occur close in time to one another. In addition, the neurons have a very low threshold of activation – they respond quickly even to weak stimuli. Finally, for FM signals, each interneuron is tuned to a specific frequency within the sweep, as well as to that same frequency in the following echo. There is specialization for the CF component of the call at this level as well. The high proportion of neurons responding to the frequency of the acoustic fovea actually increases at this level.[37][55][56]

Auditory cortex

The auditory cortex in bats is quite large in comparison with other mammals.[57] Various characteristics of sound are processed by different regions of the cortex, each providing different information about the location or movement of a target object. Most of the existing studies on information processing in the auditory cortex of the bat have been done by Nobuo Suga on the mustached bat, Pteronotus parnellii. This bat's call has both CF tone and FM sweep components.

Suga and his colleagues have shown that the cortex contains a series of "maps" of auditory information, each of which is organized systematically based on characteristics of sound such as frequency and amplitude. The neurons in these areas respond only to a specific combination of frequency and timing (sound-echo delay), and are known as combination-sensitive neurons.

The systematically organized maps in the auditory cortex respond to various aspects of the echo signal, such as its delay and its velocity. These regions are composed of "combination sensitive" neurons that require at least two specific stimuli to elicit a response. The neurons vary systematically across the maps, which are organized by acoustic features of the sound and can be two dimensional. The different features of the call and its echo are used by the bat to determine important characteristics of their prey. The maps include:

 
Auditory cortex of a bat
A FM-FM area
B CF-CF area
C Amplitude-sensitive area
D Frequency-sensitive area
E DSCF area
  • FM-FM area: This region of the cortex contains FM-FM combination-sensitive neurons. These cells respond only to the combination of two FM sweeps: a call and its echo. The neurons in the FM-FM region are often referred to as "delay-tuned," since each responds to a specific time delay between the original call and the echo, in order to find the distance from the target object (the range). Each neuron also shows specificity for one harmonic in the original call and a different harmonic in the echo. The neurons within the FM-FM area of the cortex of Pteronotus are organized into columns, in which the delay time is constant vertically but increases across the horizontal plane. The result is that range is encoded by location on the cortex, and increases systematically across the FM-FM area.[39][55][58][59]
  • CF-CF area: Another kind of combination-sensitive neuron is the CF-CF neuron. These respond best to the combination of a CF call containing two given frequencies – a call at 30 kHz (CF1) and one of its additional harmonics around 60 or 90 kHz (CF2 or CF3) – and the corresponding echoes. Thus, within the CF-CF region, the changes in echo frequency caused by the Doppler shift can be compared to the frequency of the original call to calculate the bat's velocity relative to its target object. As in the FM-FM area, information is encoded by its location within the map-like organization of the region. The CF-CF area is first split into the distinct CF1-CF2 and CF1-CF3 areas. Within each area, the CF1 frequency is organized on an axis, perpendicular to the CF2 or CF3 frequency axis. In the resulting grid, each neuron codes for a certain combination of frequencies that is indicative of a specific velocity[55][58][60]
  • Doppler shifted constant frequency (DSCF) area: This large section of the cortex is a map of the acoustic fovea, organized by frequency and by amplitude. Neurons in this region respond to CF signals that have been Doppler shifted (in other words, echoes only) and are within the same narrow frequency range to which the acoustic fovea responds. For Pteronotus, this is around 61 kHz. This area is organized into columns, which are arranged radially based on frequency. Within a column, each neuron responds to a specific combination of frequency and amplitude. Suga's studies have indicated that this brain region is necessary for frequency discrimination.[55][58][60]

Whales

 
Diagram illustrating sound generation, propagation and reception in a toothed whale. Outgoing sounds are cyan and incoming ones are green.

Biosonar is valuable to both toothed whales (suborder Odontoceti), including dolphins, porpoises, river dolphins, killer whales and sperm whales, and baleen whales (suborder Mysticeti), including right, bowhead, pygmy right, and gray whales and rorquals, because they live in an underwater habitat that has favourable acoustic characteristics and where vision is often extremely limited in range due to absorption or turbidity.[61] Odontocetes are generally able to hear sounds at ultrasonic frequencies while mysticetes hear sounds within the infrasonic frequency regime.[62]

Evolution

Cetacean evolution consisted of three main radiations. Throughout the middle and late Eocene periods (49-31.5 million years ago), archaeocetes, primitive toothed Cetacea that arose from terrestrial mammals, were the only known cetaceans.[63] These primitive aquatic mammals did not possess the ability to echolocate, although they did have slightly adapted underwater hearing.[64] The morphology of acoustically isolated ear bones in basilosaurid archaeocetes indicates that this order had directional hearing underwater at low to mid frequencies by the late middle Eocene.[65] However, with the extinction of archaeocetes at the onset of the Oligocene, two new lineages composed a second radiation. These early mysticetes (baleen whales) and odontocetes can be dated back to the middle Oligocene in New Zealand.[63] Based on past phylogenies, it has been found that extant odontocetes are monophyletic; however, echolocation evolved twice, convergently, along the odontocete lineage: once in Xenorophus, an oligocene stem odontocete, and once in the crown odontecetes [66] Dispersal rates routes of early odontocetes included transoceanic travel to new adaptive zones. The third radiation occurred later in the Neogene, when present dolphins and their relatives evolved to be the most common species in the modern sea.[64]

There are several theories to explain the evolution of echolocation. There are two proposed drives for the hypotheses of cetacean radiation, one biotic and the other abiotic. The first, adaptive radiation, is the result of a rapid divergence into new adaptive zones. This results in diverse, ecologically different clades that are incomparable.[67] Clade Neocete (crown cetacean) evolved from archaeocetes and dispersed across the world's oceans, estuaries, and rivers. These ecological opportunities were the result of abundant dietary resources with low competition for hunting.[68] This hypothesis of lineage diversification, however, can be unconvincing due to a lack of support for rapid speciation early in cetacean history. A second, more abiotic drive is better supported.[بحاجة لمصدر]

Physical restructuring of the oceans has played a role in echolocation radiation. This was a result of global climate change at the Eocene-Oligocene boundary; from a greenhouse to an icehouse world. Tectonic openings created the Southern ocean with a free flowing Antarctic Circumpolar current.[63][64][65][68] These events allowed for a selection regime characterized by the ability to locate and capture prey in turbid river waters, or allow odontocetes to invade and feed at depths below the photic zone. Further studies have found that echolocation below the photic zone could have been a predation adaptation to diel migrating cephalopods.[65][69] Since its advent, there has been adaptive radiation especially in the family Delphinidae (dolphins) in which echolocation has become extremely derived.[70]

Four proteins have been found to play a major role in toothed whale echolocation. Prestin, a motor protein of the outer hair cells of the inner ear of the mammalian cochlea, is associated with hearing sensitivity.[71] It has undergone two clear episodes of accelerated protein evolution in cetaceans.[71] The first episode of acceleration is connected to odontocete divergence, when echolocation first developed, and the second occurs with the increase in echolocation frequency seen in the family Delphinioidae. Tmc1 and Pjvk are also two proteins related to hearing sensitivity: Tmc1 is associated with hair cell development and Pjvk is associated with hair cell function.[72] Adaptations in the protein Tmc1 are related to the evolution of high-frequency hearing and molecular evolution of Tmc1 and Pjvk are indicative of positive selection for echolocation in odontocetes.[72] Cldn14, a member of the tight junction proteins which form barriers between inner ear cells, shows the same evolutionary pattern as Prestin.[73] The two events of protein evolution, for Prestin and Cldn14, occurred at the same times as the tectonic opening of the Drake Passage (34-31 Ma) and Antarctic ice growth at the Middle Miocene climate transition (14 Ma), with the divergence of odontocetes and mysticetes occurring with the former, and the speciation of Delphinioidae with the latter.[68]

The evolution of two cranial structures has been hypothesized to be linked to echolocation. Cranial telescoping developed first in xenorophids, evolving further in stem odontocetes, until the crown odontocetes show full cranial telescoping, with an overlap between the frontal and maxilla bones and a displacement of the external nares posteriorly.[74] Movement of the external nares may have allowed for a larger nasal apparatus and melon for echolocation.[74] This change occurred after the divergence of the neocetes from the basilosaurids.[75] The first shift towards cranial asymmetry occurred in the Early Oligocene, prior to the xenorophids.[75] A xenorophid fossil (Cotylocara macei) has cranial asymmetry, and shows other indicators of echolocation.[76] However, basal xenorophids lack cranial asymmetry, indicating that cranial asymmetry likely evolved twice.[75] Extant odontocetes have asymmetric nasofacial regions; generally, the median plane is shifted to the left and structures on the right are larger.[76] Both cranial telescoping and asymmetry are hypothesized to be associated with the sound production necessary for echolocation.[74]

Mechanism

Southern resident killer whales using echolocation

Thirteen species of extant odontocete evolved narrow-band high-frequency (NBHF) echolocation in four separate, convergent events. These species include the families Kogiidae (pygmy sperm whales) and Phocoenidae (porpoises), as well as some species of the genus Lagenorhynchus, all of Cephalorhynchus, and the La Plata dolphin. NBHF is thought to have evolved as a means of predator evasion; NBHF-producing species are small relative to other odontocetes, making them viable prey to large species such as the orca. However, because three of the groups developed NBHF prior to the emergence of the orca, predation by other, ancient, raptorial odontocetes must have been the driving force for the development of NBHF, not predation by the orca. Orcas, and, presumably, ancient, raptorial odontocetes such as Acrophyseter, are unable to hear frequencies below 100 kHz.[77]

Another reason for variation in echolocation is habitat. For all sonar systems the limiting factor deciding whether a returning echo is detected is the echo-to-noise ratio (ENR). The ENR is given by the emitted source level (SL) plus the target strength, minus the two-way transmission loss (absorption and spreading) and the received noise.[78] Animals will adapt either to maximize range under noise-limited conditions (increase source level) or to reduce noise clutter in a shallow and/or littered habitat (decrease source level). In cluttered habitats, such as coastal areas, prey ranges are smaller, and species such as Commerson's dolphin (Cephalorhynchus commersonii) have lowered source levels to better suit their environment.[78]

Toothed whales emit a focused beam of high-frequency clicks in the direction that their head is pointing. Sounds are generated by passing air from the bony nares through the phonic lips. These sounds are reflected by the dense concave bone of the cranium and an air sac at its base. The focused beam is modulated by a large fatty organ known as the 'melon'. This acts like an acoustic lens because it is composed of lipids of differing densities. Most toothed whales use clicks in a series, or click train, for echolocation, while the sperm whale may produce clicks individually. Toothed whale whistles do not appear to be used in echolocation. Different rates of click production in a click train give rise to the familiar barks, squeals and growls of the bottlenose dolphin. A click train with a repetition rate over 600 per second is called a burst pulse. In bottlenose dolphins, the auditory brain response resolves individual clicks up to 600 per second, but yields a graded response for higher repetition rates.[79]

It has been suggested that some smaller toothed whales may have their tooth arrangement suited to aid in echolocation. The placement of teeth in the jaw of a bottlenose dolphin, for example, are not symmetrical when seen from a vertical plane, and this asymmetry could possibly be an aid in the dolphin sensing if echoes from its biosonar are coming from one side or the other.[80][81] However, this idea lacks experimental support.

Echoes are received using complex fatty structures around the lower jaw as the primary reception path, from where they are transmitted to the middle ear via a continuous fat body. Lateral sound may be received through fatty lobes surrounding the ears with a similar density to water. Some researchers believe that when they approach the object of interest, they protect themselves against the louder echo by quietening the emitted sound. In bats this is known to happen, but here the hearing sensitivity is also reduced close to a target.[82][83]

Oilbirds and swiftlets

 
A Palawan swiftlet (Aerodramus palawanensis) flies in complete darkness inside the Puerto Princesa subterranean river cave.

Oilbirds and some species of swiftlet are known to use a relatively crude form of echolocation compared to that of bats and dolphins. These nocturnal birds emit calls while flying and use the calls to navigate through trees and caves where they live.[84][85]

Shrews, tenrecs and rats

Terrestrial mammals other than bats known or thought to echolocate include two shrew genera (Sorex and Blarina), the tenrecs of Madagascar and solenodons.[86][87] These include the wandering shrew (Sorex vagrans), the common or Eurasian shrew (Sorex araneus), and the short-tailed shrew (Blarina brevicauda).[88] The nature of shrew sounds unlike those of bats are low amplitude, broadband, multi-harmonic and frequency modulated.[88] They contain no 'echolocation clicks' with reverberations and would seem to be used for simple, close range spatial orientation. In contrast to bats, shrews use echolocation only to investigate their habitat rather than additionally to pinpoint food.[88]

There is evidence that blinded laboratory rats can use echolocation to navigate mazes.[89]

Echolocation jamming

Echolocation systems are susceptible to interference known as echolocation jamming or sonar jamming. Jamming occurs when non-target sounds interfere with target echoes. Jamming can be purposeful or inadvertent and can be caused by the echolocation system itself, other echolocating animals,[90] prey,[91] or humans. Echolocating animals have evolved to minimize jamming; however, echolocation avoidance behaviors are not always successful.

Background noise can also interfere with echolocation-based tasks via masking - a mechanism whereby noise interferes with the auditory perception of a signal - and via distraction - a mechanism whereby animals allocate finite attentional resources to noise rather than the signal.[92]

Avoidance by prey animals

Galleria mellonella exhibits predator avoidance behaviors such as dropping, looping, and freezing when emitters sent out ultrasound waves, indicating that G. mellonella can both detect and differentiate between ultrasound frequencies used by predators or from other members of their species.[93] Members of the Saturniidae moth family, which include giant silk moths, have been observed using their large wings to deflect the ultrasonic signals of microbats.[94]

انظر أيضاً

المصادر

  1. ^ Teeling EC, Jones G, Rossiter SJ (2016). "Phylogeny, Genes, and Hearing: Implications for the Evolution of Echolocation in Bats". In Fenton MB, Grinnell AD, Popper AN, Fay RN (eds.). Bat Bioacoustics. Springer Handbook of Auditory Research. Vol. 54. New York: Springer. pp. 25–54. doi:10.1007/978-1-4939-3527-7_2. ISBN 9781493935277.
  2. ^ Tsagkogeorga G, Parker J, Stupka E, Cotton JA, Rossiter SJ (November 2013). "Phylogenomic analyses elucidate the evolutionary relationships of bats". Current Biology. 23 (22): 2262–2267. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.09.014. PMID 24184098.
  3. ^ Yoon CK (14 November 2003). "Donald R. Griffin, 88, Dies; Argued Animals Can Think". The New York Times.
  4. ^ Griffin DR (December 1944). "Echolocation by Blind Men, Bats and Radar". Science. 100 (2609): 589–90. Bibcode:1944Sci...100..589G. doi:10.1126/science.100.2609.589. PMID 17776129.
  5. ^ Griffin, D. R. (1958). Listening in the dark. Yale University Press.
  6. ^ Spallanzani, Lazzaro (1794). Lettere sopra il sospetto di un nuovo senso nei pipistrelli [Letters on the suspicion of a new sense in bats] (in الإيطالية). Turin, Italy: Stamperia Reale.
  7. ^ Dijkgraaf, Sven (March 1960). "Spallanzani's unpublished experiments on the sensory basis of object perception in bats". Isis. 51 (1): 9–20. doi:10.1086/348834. PMID 13816753. S2CID 11923119.
  8. ^ Peschier (1798). "Extraits des expériences de Jurine sur les chauve-souris qu'on a privé de la vue" [Extracts of Jurine's experiments on bats that have been deprived of sight]. Journal de physique, de chimie, d'histoire naturelle (in الفرنسية). 46: 145–148.
  9. ^ De Jurine, M. (1798). "Experiments on bats deprived of sight". Philosophical Magazine. 1 (2): 136–140. doi:10.1080/14786447808676811. From p. 140: From these experiments the author concludes: … that the organ of hearing appears to supply that of sight in the discovery of bodies, and to furnish these animals with different sensations to direct their flight, and enable them to avoid those obstacles which may present themselves.
  10. ^ Dijkgraaf, S. (1949). "Spallanzani und die Fledermäuse" [Spallanzani and the bat]. Experientia. 5 (2): 90–92. doi:10.1007/bf02153744. S2CID 500691.
  11. ^ Hahn WL (1908). "Some habits and sensory adaptations of cave-inhabiting bats". Biological Bulletin. 15 (3): 135–198, especially pp. 165–178. doi:10.2307/1536066. JSTOR 1536066.
  12. ^ Maxim H (7 September 1912). "The sixth sense of the bat. Sir Hiram Maxim's contention. The possible prevention of sea collisions". Scientific American Supplement. 74: 148–150.
  13. ^ Hartridge H (1920). "The avoidance of objects by bats in their flight". Journal of Physiology. 54 (1–2): 54–57. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1920.sp001908. PMC 1405739. PMID 16993475.
  14. ^ Thorpe (1958). "Review of "Listening in the Dark". Science. 128 (3327): 766. doi:10.1126/science.128.3327.766. JSTOR 1754799.
  15. ^ Schevill WE, McBride AF (1956). "Evidence for echolocation by cetaceans". Deep-Sea Research. 3 (2): 153–154. Bibcode:1956DSR.....3..153S. doi:10.1016/0146-6313(56)90096-x.
  16. ^ The Silent World, pp. 206–207
  17. ^ Jones G (July 2005). "Echolocation". Current Biology. 15 (13): R484-8. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.06.051. PMID 16005275. S2CID 235311777.
  18. ^ Lewanzik D, Sundaramurthy AK, Goerlitz HR (October 2019). Derryberry E (ed.). "Insectivorous bats integrate social information about species identity, conspecific activity and prey abundance to estimate cost-benefit ratio of interactions". The Journal of Animal Ecology. 88 (10): 1462–1473. doi:10.1111/1365-2656.12989. PMC 6849779. PMID 30945281.
  19. ^ Shriram U, Simmons JA (June 2019). "Echolocating bats perceive natural-size targets as a unitary class using micro-spectral ripples in echoes". Behavioral Neuroscience. 133 (3): 297–304. doi:10.1037/bne0000315. PMID 31021108.
  20. ^ Syka J, Masterton RB (2012-12-06). Auditory Pathway: Structure and Function. Springer. ISBN 978-1-468-41300-7.
  21. ^ Lima SL, O'Keefe JM (August 2013). "Do predators influence the behaviour of bats?". Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society. 88 (3): 626–44. doi:10.1111/brv.12021. PMID 23347323. S2CID 32118961.
  22. ^ Basic Biology (2015). "Bats".
  23. ^ Müller R (December 2004). "A numerical study of the role of the tragus in the big brown bat". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 116 (6): 3701–12. Bibcode:2004ASAJ..116.3701M. doi:10.1121/1.1815133. PMID 15658720.
  24. ^ Teeling EC, Scally M, Kao DJ, Romagnoli ML, Springer MS, Stanhope MJ (January 2000). "Molecular evidence regarding the origin of echolocation and flight in bats". Nature. 403 (6766): 188–92. Bibcode:2000Natur.403..188T. doi:10.1038/35003188. PMID 10646602. S2CID 205004782.
  25. ^ "Order Chiroptera (Bats)". Animal Diversity Web. Archived from the original on 21 December 2007. Retrieved 2007-12-30.
  26. ^ Nojiri, Taro; Wilson, Laura A. B.; López-Aguirre, Camilo; Tu, Vuong Tan; Kuratani, Shigeru; Ito, Kai; Higashiyama, Hiroki; Son, Nguyen Truong; Fukui, Dai; Sadier, Alexa; Sears, Karen E. (2021-04-12). "Embryonic evidence uncovers convergent origins of laryngeal echolocation in bats". Current Biology (in English). 31 (7): 1353–1365.e3. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2020.12.043. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 33675700.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unrecognized language (link)
  27. ^ Jebb, David; Huang, Zixia; Pippel, Martin; Hughes, Graham M.; Lavrichenko, Ksenia; Devanna, Paolo; Winkler, Sylke; Jermiin, Lars S.; Skirmuntt, Emilia C.; Katzourakis, Aris; Burkitt-Gray, Lucy (July 2020). "Six reference-quality genomes reveal evolution of bat adaptations". Nature (in الإنجليزية). 583 (7817): 578–584. doi:10.1038/s41586-020-2486-3. ISSN 1476-4687. PMC 8075899. PMID 32699395.
  28. ^ Wang, Zhe; Zhu, Tengteng; Xue, Huiling; Fang, Na; Zhang, Junpeng; Zhang, Libiao; Pang, Jian; Teeling, Emma C.; Zhang, Shuyi (2017-01-09). "Prenatal development supports a single origin of laryngeal echolocation in bats". Nature Ecology & Evolution (in الإنجليزية). 1 (2): 1–5. doi:10.1038/s41559-016-0021. ISSN 2397-334X.
  29. ^ Springer MS, Teeling EC, Madsen O, Stanhope MJ, de Jong WW (May 2001). "Integrated fossil and molecular data reconstruct bat echolocation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 98 (11): 6241–6. Bibcode:2001PNAS...98.6241S. doi:10.1073/pnas.111551998. PMC 33452. PMID 11353869.
  30. ^ "Wyoming Bat Call Library". University of Wyoming. Retrieved 16 January 2019.
  31. ^ "Pacific Northwest Bat Call Library". University of Washington. Retrieved 16 January 2019.
  32. ^ Fukui D, Agetsuma N, Hill DA (September 2004). "Acoustic identification of eight species of bat (mammalia: chiroptera) inhabiting forests of southern hokkaido, Japan: potential for conservation monitoring" (PDF). Zoological Science. 21 (9): 947–55. doi:10.2108/zsj.21.947. hdl:2115/14484. PMID 15459453. S2CID 20190217.
  33. ^ Speakman JR, Racey PA (April 1991). "No cost of echolocation for bats in flight". Nature. 350 (6317): 421–3. Bibcode:1991Natur.350..421S. doi:10.1038/350421a0. PMID 2011191. S2CID 4314715.
  34. ^ Gordon SD, Ter Hofstede HM (March 2018). "The influence of bat echolocation call duration and timing on auditory encoding of predator distance in noctuoid moths". The Journal of Experimental Biology. 221 (Pt 6): jeb171561. doi:10.1242/jeb.171561. PMID 29567831.
  35. ^ أ ب ت ث ج ح Jones G, Teeling EC (March 2006). "The evolution of echolocation in bats". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 21 (3): 149–56. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2006.01.001. PMID 16701491.
  36. ^ أ ب ت ث ج Grinnell AD (1995). "Hearing in Bats: An Overview.". In Popper AN, Fay RR (eds.). Hearing in Bats. New York: Springer Verlag. pp. 1–36.
  37. ^ أ ب ت ث ج Zupanc GK (2004). Behavioral Neurobiology: An Integrative Approach. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  38. ^ أ ب ت ث ج Fenton MB (1995). "Natural History and Biosonar Signals". In Popper AN, d Fay RR (eds.). Hearing in Bats. New York: Springer Verlag. pp. 37–86.
  39. ^ أ ب ت ث ج ح Neuweiler G (2003). "Evolutionary aspects of bat echolocation". J. Comp. Physiol. A. 189 (4): 245–256. doi:10.1007/s00359-003-0406-2. PMID 12743729. S2CID 8761216.
  40. ^ أ ب ت ث ج ح Simmons JA, Stein RA (1980). "Acoustic Imaging in bat sonar: echolocation signals and the evolution of echolocation". J. Comp. Physiol. A. 135 (1): 61–84. doi:10.1007/bf00660182. S2CID 20515827.
  41. ^ Fullard JH (1998). "The Sensory Coevolution of Moths and Bats". In Hoy RR, Popper AN, Fay RR (eds.). Comparative Hearing: Insects. Springer Handbook of Auditory Research. Vol. 10. New York: Springer. pp. 279–326. doi:10.1007/978-1-4612-0585-2_8. ISBN 9781461205852.
  42. ^ Goerlitz HR, ter Hofstede HM, Zeale MR, Jones G, Holderied MW (September 2010). "An aerial-hawking bat uses stealth echolocation to counter moth hearing". Current Biology. 20 (17): 1568–72. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.07.046. PMID 20727755.
  43. ^ Ratcliffe JM, Elemans CP, Jakobsen L, Surlykke A (April 2013). "How the bat got its buzz". Biology Letters. 9 (2): 20121031. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2012.1031. PMC 3639754. PMID 23302868.
  44. ^ Fenton 2005
  45. ^ أ ب Hiryu S, Hagino T, Riquimaroux H, Watanabe Y (March 2007). "Echo-intensity compensation in echolocating bats (Pipistrellus abramus) during flight measured by a telemetry microphone". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 121 (3): 1749–57. Bibcode:2007ASAJ..121.1749H. doi:10.1121/1.2431337. PMID 17407911. S2CID 14511456.
  46. ^ أ ب Jones G, Holderied MW (April 2007). "Bat echolocation calls: adaptation and convergent evolution". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 274 (1612): 905–12. doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.0200. PMC 1919403. PMID 17251105.
  47. ^ Fenton MB, Portfors CV, Rautenbach IL, Waterman JM (1998). "Compromises: Sound frequencies used in echolocation by aerial-feeding bats". Canadian Journal of Zoology. 76 (6): 1174–1182. doi:10.1139/cjz-76-6-1174.
  48. ^ أ ب Fullard J, Dawson J (1997). "The echolocation calls of the spotted bat Euderma maculatum are relatively inaudible to moths". The Journal of Experimental Biology. 200 (Pt 1): 129–37. doi:10.1242/jeb.200.1.129. PMID 9317482.
  49. ^ Surlykke A, Kalko EK (April 2008). "Echolocating bats cry out loud to detect their prey". PLOS ONE. 3 (4): e2036. Bibcode:2008PLoSO...3.2036S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002036. PMC 2323577. PMID 18446226.
  50. ^ Holderied MW, von Helversen O (November 2003). "Echolocation range and wingbeat period match in aerial-hawking bats". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 270 (1530): 2293–9. doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2487. PMC 1691500. PMID 14613617.
  51. ^ Wilson W, Moss C (2004). Thomas J, Moss C (eds.). Echolocation in Bats and Dolphins. p. 22.
  52. ^ Schnitzler HU, Flieger E (1983). "Detection of oscillating target movements by echolocation in the Greater Horseshoe bat". J. Comp. Physiol. 153 (3): 385–391. doi:10.1007/bf00612592. S2CID 36824634.
  53. ^ Salles A, Bohn KM, Moss CF (June 2019). "Auditory communication processing in bats: What we know and where to go". Behavioral Neuroscience. 133 (3): 305–319. doi:10.1037/bne0000308. PMID 31045392. S2CID 143423009.
  54. ^ Schuller G, Pollack G (1979). "Disproportionate frequency representation in the inferior colliculus of Doppler-compensating greater horseshoe bats: Evidence of an acoustic fovea". Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 132 (1): 47–54. doi:10.1007/bf00617731. S2CID 7176515.
  55. ^ أ ب ت ث ج Carew T (2001). Behavioral Neurobiology. USA: Sinauer Associates, Inc.
  56. ^ Pollak G, Marsh D, Bodenhamer R, Souther A (May 1977). "Echo-detecting characteristics of neurons in inferior colliculus of unanesthetized bats". Science. 196 (4290): 675–8. Bibcode:1977Sci...196..675P. doi:10.1126/science.857318. PMID 857318.
  57. ^ Kanwal JS, Rauschecker JP (May 2007). "Auditory cortex of bats and primates: managing species-specific calls for social communication". Frontiers in Bioscience. 12 (8–12): 4621–40. doi:10.2741/2413. PMC 4276140. PMID 17485400.
  58. ^ أ ب ت Suga N, Simmons JA, Jen PH (1975). "Peripheral specialization for fine analysis of doppler-shifted echoes in the auditory system of the "CF-FM" bat Pteronotus parnellii". Journal of Experimental Biology. 63 (1): 161–192. doi:10.1242/jeb.63.1.161. PMID 1159359.
  59. ^ Suga et al. 1979
  60. ^ أ ب Suga N, Niwa H, Taniguchi I, Margoliash D (October 1987). "The personalized auditory cortex of the mustached bat: adaptation for echolocation". Journal of Neurophysiology. 58 (4): 643–54. doi:10.1152/jn.1987.58.4.643. PMID 3681389. S2CID 18390219.
  61. ^ Hughes, H.C. (1999). Sensory Exotica : A World Beyond Human Experience. Cambridge, Mass: A Bradford Book.
  62. ^ Viglino, M.; Gaetán, M.; Buono, M.R.; Fordyce, R.E.; Park, T. (2021). "Hearing from the ocean and into the river: the evolution of the inner ear of Platanistoidea (Cetacea: Odontoceti)". Paleobiology: 1–21. doi:10.1017/pab.2021.11.
  63. ^ أ ب ت Fordyce, R. E. (1980). "Whale evolution and oligocene southern-ocean environments". Palaeogeography, Palaeoclimatology, Palaeoecology. 31: 319–336. Bibcode:1980PPP....31..319F. doi:10.1016/0031-0182(80)90024-3.
  64. ^ أ ب ت Fordyce, R. E. 2003. Cetacean evolution and Eocene-Oligocene oceans revisited. From greenhouse to icehouse: the marine eocene-oligocene transition:154–170.
  65. ^ أ ب ت Lindberg DR, Pyenson ND (2007). "Things that go bump in the night: evolutionary interactions between cephalopods and cetaceans in the tertiary". Lethaia. 40 (4): 335–343. doi:10.1111/j.1502-3931.2007.00032.x.
  66. ^ Racicot RA, Boessenecker RW, Darroch SA, Geisler JH (2019). "Evidence for convergent evolution of ultrasonic hearing in toothed whales". Biology Letters. 15 (5): 20190083. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2019.0083. PMC 6548736. PMID 31088283. S2CID 155091623.
  67. ^ Slater GJ, Price SA, Santini F, Alfaro ME (October 2010). "Diversity versus disparity and the radiation of modern cetaceans". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 277 (1697): 3097–104. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.0408. PMC 2982053. PMID 20484243.
  68. ^ أ ب ت Steeman ME, Hebsgaard MB, Fordyce RE, Ho SY, Rabosky DL, Nielsen R, et al. (December 2009). "Radiation of extant cetaceans driven by restructuring of the oceans". Systematic Biology. 58 (6): 573–85. doi:10.1093/sysbio/syp060. PMC 2777972. PMID 20525610.
  69. ^ Fordyce RE, Barnes LG (1994). "The evolutionary history of whales and dolphins". Annual Review of Earth and Planetary Sciences. 22 (1): 419–455. Bibcode:1994AREPS..22..419F. doi:10.1146/annurev.ea.22.050194.002223.
  70. ^ McGowen MR, Spaulding M, Gatesy J (December 2009). "Divergence date estimation and a comprehensive molecular tree of extant cetaceans". Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution. 53 (3): 891–906. doi:10.1016/j.ympev.2009.08.018. PMID 19699809.
  71. ^ أ ب Liu Y, Rossiter SJ, Han X, Cotton JA, Zhang S (October 2010). "Cetaceans on a molecular fast track to ultrasonic hearing". Current Biology. 20 (20): 1834–9. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.008. PMID 20933423.
  72. ^ أ ب Davies, K. T. J.; Cotton, J. A.; Kirwan, J. D.; Teeling, E. C.; Rossiter, S. J. (May 2012). "Parallel signatures of sequence evolution among hearing genes in echolocating mammals: an emerging model of genetic convergence". Heredity (in الإنجليزية). 108 (5): 480–489. doi:10.1038/hdy.2011.119. ISSN 1365-2540. PMC 3330687. PMID 22167055.
  73. ^ Xu H, Liu Y, He G, Rossiter SJ, Zhang S (November 2013). "Adaptive evolution of tight junction protein claudin-14 in echolocating whales". Gene. 530 (2): 208–14. doi:10.1016/j.gene.2013.08.034. PMID 23965379.
  74. ^ أ ب ت Churchill, Morgan; Geisler, Jonathan H.; Beatty, Brian L.; Goswami, Anjali (2018). "Evolution of cranial telescoping in echolocating whales (Cetacea: Odontoceti)". Evolution (in الإنجليزية). 72 (5): 1092–1108. doi:10.1111/evo.13480. ISSN 1558-5646.
  75. ^ أ ب ت Coombs, Ellen J.; Clavel, Julien; Park, Travis; Churchill, Morgan; Goswami, Anjali (2020-07-10). "Wonky whales: the evolution of cranial asymmetry in cetaceans". BMC Biology. 18 (1): 86. doi:10.1186/s12915-020-00805-4. ISSN 1741-7007. PMC 7350770. PMID 32646447.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  76. ^ أ ب Geisler, Jonathan H.; Colbert, Matthew W.; Carew, James L. (April 2014). "A new fossil species supports an early origin for toothed whale echolocation". Nature (in الإنجليزية). 508 (7496): 383–386. doi:10.1038/nature13086. ISSN 1476-4687.
  77. ^ Galatius A, Olsen MT, Steeman ME, Racicot RA, Bradshaw CD, Kyhn LA, Miller LA (2019). "Raising your voice: evolution of narrow-band high-frequency signals in toothed whales (Odontoceti)". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 166 (2): 213–224. doi:10.1093/biolinnean/bly194.
  78. ^ أ ب Kyhn LA, Jensen FH, Beedholm K, Tougaard J, Hansen M, Madsen PT (June 2010). "Echolocation in sympatric Peale's dolphins (Lagenorhynchus australis) and Commerson's dolphins (Cephalorhynchus commersonii) producing narrow-band high-frequency clicks". The Journal of Experimental Biology. 213 (11): 1940–9. doi:10.1242/jeb.042440. PMID 20472781.
  79. ^ Cranford TW (2000). "In Search of Impulse Sound Sources in Odontocetes.". In Au WW, Popper AN, Fay RR (eds.). Hearing by Whales and Dolphins. Springer Handbook of Auditory Research series. New York: Springer-Verlag.
  80. ^ Goodson AD, Klinowska MA (1990). "A proposed echolocation receptor for the Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): modeling the receive directivity from tooth and lower jaw geometry". In Thomas JA, Kastelein RA (eds.). Sensory Abilities of Cetaceans. NATO ASI Series A. Vol. 196. New York: Plenum. pp. 255–67.
  81. ^ Dobbins P (March 2007). "Dolphin sonar--modelling a new receiver concept" (PDF). Bioinspiration & Biomimetics. 2 (1): 19–29. Bibcode:2007BiBi....2...19D. doi:10.1088/1748-3182/2/1/003. PMID 17671323.
  82. ^ Ketten DR (1992). "The Marine Mammal Ear: Specializations for aquatic audition and echolocation.". In Webster D, Fay R, Popper A (eds.). The Evolutionary Biology of Hearing. Springer-Verlag. pp. 717–750.
  83. ^ Ketten DR (2000). "Cetacean Ears". In Au W, Fay R, Popper A (eds.). Hearing by Whales and Dolphins. SHAR Series for Auditory Research. Springer-Verlag. pp. 43–108.
  84. ^ Fjeldså J, Krabbe N (1990). Birds of the High Andes: a manual to the birds of the temperate zone of the Andes and Patagonia, South America. Apollo Books. p. 232. ISBN 978-87-88757-16-3.
  85. ^ Marshall Cavendish (2000). Exploring Life Biology. Marshall Cavendish. p. 547. ISBN 978-0-7614-7142-4.
  86. ^ Tomasi, Thomas E. (1979). "Echolocation by the Short-Tailed Shrew Blarina brevicauda". Journal of Mammalogy. 60 (4): 751–759. doi:10.2307/1380190. JSTOR 1380190.
  87. ^ Eisenberg, J. F.; Gould, E. (1966). "The Behavior of Solenodon paradoxus in Captivity with Comments on the Behavior of other Insectivora" (PDF). Zoologica. 51 (4): 49–60. hdl:10088/11117. Retrieved 8 سبتمبر 2020.
  88. ^ أ ب ت Siemers BM, Schauermann G, Turni H, von Merten S (October 2009). "Why do shrews twitter? Communication or simple echo-based orientation". Biology Letters. 5 (5): 593–6. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.0378. PMC 2781971. PMID 19535367.
  89. ^ Riley DA, Rosenzweig MR (August 1957). "Echolocation in rats". Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 50 (4): 323–8. doi:10.1037/h0047398. PMID 13475510.
  90. ^ Chiu C, Xian W, Moss CF (September 2008). "Flying in silence: Echolocating bats cease vocalizing to avoid sonar jamming". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 105 (35): 13116–21. Bibcode:2008PNAS..10513116C. doi:10.1073/pnas.0804408105. PMC 2529029. PMID 18725624.
  91. ^ Barber JR, Conner WE (July 2006). "Tiger moth responses to a simulated bat attack: timing and duty cycle". The Journal of Experimental Biology. 209 (Pt 14): 2637–50. doi:10.1242/jeb.02295. PMID 16809455.
  92. ^ Gomes, Dylan G. E.; Goerlitz, Holger R. (18 December 2020). "Individual differences show that only some bats can cope with noise-induced masking and distraction". PeerJ (in الإنجليزية). 8: e10551. doi:10.7717/peerj.10551. ISSN 2167-8359. PMC 7751433. PMID 33384901.
  93. ^ Spangler HG (1988). "Sound and the Moths That Infest Beehives". The Florida Entomologist. 71 (4): 467–477. doi:10.2307/3495006. JSTOR 3495006.
  94. ^ Rubin JJ, Hamilton CA, McClure CJ, Chadwell BA, Kawahara AY, Barber JR (July 2018). "The evolution of anti-bat sensory illusions in moths". Science Advances. 4 (7): eaar7428. Bibcode:2018SciA....4.7428R. doi:10.1126/sciadv.aar7428. PMC 6031379. PMID 29978042.

قراءات إضافية

وصلات خارجية